记忆容量选择的控制和价值取向在正常儿童与多动症儿童间的差异毕业论文外文翻译.doc
《记忆容量选择的控制和价值取向在正常儿童与多动症儿童间的差异毕业论文外文翻译.doc》由会员分享,可在线阅读,更多相关《记忆容量选择的控制和价值取向在正常儿童与多动症儿童间的差异毕业论文外文翻译.doc(22页珍藏版)》请在淘文阁 - 分享文档赚钱的网站上搜索。
1、Memory Capacity, Selective Control, and Value-Directed Remembering in Children With and Without Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)Alan D. Castel, Steve S. Lee, and Kathryn L. Humphreys University of California, Los Angeles Amy N. MoorePennsylvania State UniversityObjective: The ability
2、to select what is important to remember, to attend to this information, and to recall high-value items leads to the efficient use of memory. The present study examined how children with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) performed on an incentive-based selectivity task in wh
3、ich to-be-remembered items were worth different point values. Method: Participants were 6-9 year old children with ADHD (n = 57) and without ADHD (n = 59). Using a selectivity task, participants studied words paired with point values and were asked to maximize their score, which was the overall valu
4、e of the items they recalled. This task allows for measures of memory capacity and the ability to selectively remember high-value items. Results: Although there were no significant between-groups differences in the number of words recalled (memory capacity), children with ADHD were less selective th
5、an children in the control group in terms of the value of the items they recalled (control of memory). All children recalled more high-value items than low-value items and showed some learning with task experience, but children with ADHD Combined type did not efficiently maximize memory performance
6、(as measured by a selectivity index) relative to children with ADHD Inattentive type and healthy controls, who did not differ significantly from one another. Conclusions: Children with ADHD Combined type exhibit impairments in the strategic and efficient encoding and recall of high-value items. The
7、findings have implications for theories of memory dysfunction in childhood ADHD and the key role of metacognition, cognitive control, and value-directed remembering when considering the strategic use of memory. Keywords: ADHD, memory, cognitivecontrol, metamemory, encoding strategiesAttention-defici
8、t/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is characterized by an early onset of developmentally aberrant and impairing levels of inattention and/or hyperactivity -impulsivity (American Psychiatric Association APA, 2000). In addition to its concurrent and prospective association with disrupted social/family re
9、lationships, substandard academic achievement, and elevated comorbidity (Barkley, Fischer, Edelbrock, & Smallish, 1990; Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2002; Lee, Lahey, Owens, & Hinshaw, 2008; Owens, Hinshaw, Lee, & Lahey, 2009), ADHD is also associated with neuropsychological deficits acro
10、ss domains such as cognitive flexibility, problem solving, and working memory (Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005). Working memory (WM) involves the active maintenance and manipulation of information, and is governed by executive control processes (Baddeley, 1992, 2007). A recent met
11、a-analysis showed that children with ADHD have specific, robust deficits in WM, which are more pronounced in spatial WM tasks than in verbal WM tasks (Martinussen, Hayden, Hogg-Johnson, & Tannock, 2005). Furthermore, there is evidence that neuropsychological deficits, including problems with WM, par
12、tially mediate the persistence of ADHD over time, as well as the degree of functional impairment associated with ADHD (Halperin, Trampush, Miller, Mark, & Newcorn, 2008). & NewcornFor example, ADHD is associated with ineffective use of memory strategies and/or a failure to sustain effortful processi
13、ng over time (ONeill & Douglas, 1996). This may also be related to impairments in goal maintenance (Kane & Engle, 2003). A great deal of research shows that inhibitory control changes dramatically across the life span (Bedard, Nichols, Barbosa, Schachar, Logan, & Tannock, 2002; Zelazo, Craik, & Boot
14、h, 2004), and that children with ADHD have specific deficits in inhibitory control (Barkley,1997). However, most research on WM, inhibition, and executive control has used tasks that do not provide strong incentives to selectively focus on and remember important or high value information, at the cos
15、t of lower-value information. Typically WM tasks do not discriminate items by their relative importance and WM performance is operationalized by how many items are retained. The present study expands on the literature on WM by examining how children with and without ADHD strategically focus on and r
16、etain high-value information in WM and how this ability changes with task experience.The “selectivity task,” a relatively novel method for examining how people can selectively encode and maintain high-value information, differs from traditional measures of WM in that it investigates how one selectiv
17、ely encodes information using strategic control, and has now been used in several studies with various populations (see Castel, Benjamin, Craik, & Watkins, 2002; Castel, Farb, & Craik, 2007; Hanten, Li, Chapman, Swank, Gamino, Roberson, & Levin, 2007; Watkins & Bloom, 1999). In the present study, we
18、 used a modified form of this paradigm, in which words with different values (e.g., points) were to be remembered by the participant. This procedure allows one to examine the extent to which people use value-based information to guide the efficient use of memory (e.g., by intentionally recalling hig
19、her valued items). The point value assigned to each item during encoding indicates how important each item is to remember. This task differs from traditional measures of episodic memory or common tests of WM span as it examines the strategic control of encoding high-value information. Whereas this v
20、alue-directed remembering approach may share some resources with WM function, the selectivity task specifically allows for an examination of the strategic deployment of memory capacity, and the awareness of limited memory capacity (which can be conceptualized as a form of metamemory).In the selectiv
21、ity paradigm, participants are presented with lists of words, with each word in the list having a distinct value ranging from 1 to 12 points. Participants are instructed to remember as many words as possible, with the goal of maximizing their score, which is the sum of the point values of each recal
22、led word. After recall,participants are told their score, and then are given a new list, with instructions to maximize their score. Using a selectivity index (SI) developed by Watkins and Bloom (1999; see also Hanten et al., 2007), we examined how selectivity changed with task experience. This SI is
23、 based on the participants score (the sum of the points that were paired with therecalled items, or the “value” of the recalled items), relative to chance and ideal performance. The equation accounts for the participants score relative to an ideal score that represents recall of only the most highly
24、 valued words at that level of recall. For example, if a given participant remembered four words, and the points associated with the words were 12, 10, 9, and 8, that participants SI would be considered quite high. The ideal score for four words is 12 +11 +10 +9 = 42, whereas the score of the partic
- 配套讲稿:
如PPT文件的首页显示word图标,表示该PPT已包含配套word讲稿。双击word图标可打开word文档。
- 特殊限制:
部分文档作品中含有的国旗、国徽等图片,仅作为作品整体效果示例展示,禁止商用。设计者仅对作品中独创性部分享有著作权。
- 关 键 词:
- 记忆容量 选择的控制 和价值取向 在正常儿童与多动症儿童间的差异 毕业论文外文翻译 记忆 容量 选择 控制 价值 取向 正常 儿童 多动症 差异 毕业论文 外文 翻译
限制150内